Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
: 20 | 50 | 100
1 - 20 de 4.556
1.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 10513, 2024 05 07.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38714675

In the current research, we used data from a sample of 16,327 menstrual cycle tracking app users to examine the association between menstrual cycle characteristics and sexual motivation tracked over 10 months of app use. Guided by past work that finds links between menstrual cycle characteristics related to conception risk and sexual motivation, we found that (a) between-women, shorter (r = - 0.04, p = 0.007), more regular cycles predicted small increases in sexual motivation (r = - 0.04, p = 0.001); (b) within-women, shorter cycles predicted greater sexual motivation that month (r = - 0.04, p < 0.001) and (c) the next month (ßs: - 0.10 to - 0.06, ps < 0.001), but (d) changes in sexual motivation did not reliably precede changes in cycle length (ßs: - 0.01 to 0.02, ps > 0.15). Within-woman analyses also revealed that (e) shorter cycles were followed by more frequent reports of fatigue (ß = - 0.06, p < 0.001), insomnia (ß = - 0.03, p < 0.001), and food cravings (ß = - 0.04, p < 0.001). Together, results suggest that menstrual cycles characteristics and sexual motivation may covary together in ways that reflect changing investments in reproduction. Small effect sizes and lack of experimental control warrant cautious interpretations of results.


Menstrual Cycle , Motivation , Sexual Behavior , Humans , Female , Menstrual Cycle/physiology , Menstrual Cycle/psychology , Motivation/physiology , Sexual Behavior/psychology , Sexual Behavior/physiology , Adult , Longitudinal Studies , Young Adult , Mobile Applications , Adolescent
2.
Sensors (Basel) ; 24(9)2024 Apr 30.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38732962

Being motivated has positive influences on task performance. However, motivation could result from various motives that affect different parts of the brain. Analyzing the motivation effect from all affected areas requires a high number of EEG electrodes, resulting in high cost, inflexibility, and burden to users. In various real-world applications, only the motivation effect is required for performance evaluation regardless of the motive. Analyzing the relationships between the motivation-affected brain areas associated with the task's performance could limit the required electrodes. This study introduced a method to identify the cognitive motivation effect with a reduced number of EEG electrodes. The temporal association rule mining (TARM) concept was used to analyze the relationships between attention and memorization brain areas under the effect of motivation from the cognitive motivation task. For accuracy improvement, the artificial bee colony (ABC) algorithm was applied with the central limit theorem (CLT) concept to optimize the TARM parameters. From the results, our method can identify the motivation effect with only FCz and P3 electrodes, with 74.5% classification accuracy on average with individual tests.


Algorithms , Cognition , Electroencephalography , Motivation , Motivation/physiology , Electroencephalography/methods , Humans , Cognition/physiology , Male , Adult , Female , Brain/physiology , Young Adult , Electrodes , Data Mining/methods
3.
Behav Brain Res ; 466: 114977, 2024 May 28.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38570074

Apathy is a complex psychiatric syndrome characterised by motivational deficit, emotional blunting and cognitive changes. It occurs alongside a broad range of neurological disorders, but also occurs in otherwise healthy ageing. Despite its clinical prevalence, apathy does not yet have a designated treatment strategy. Generation of a translational animal model of apathy syndrome would facilitate the development of novel treatments. Given the multidimensional nature of apathy, a model cannot be achieved with a single behavioural test. Using a battery of behavioural tests we investigated whether aged rats exhibit behavioural deficits across different domains relevant to apathy. Using the effort for reward and progressive ratio tasks we found that aged male rats (21-27 months) show intact reward motivation. Using the novelty supressed feeding test and position-based object exploration we found aged rats showed increased anxiety-like behaviour inconsistent with emotional blunting. The sucrose preference test and reward learning assay showed intact reward sensitivity and reward-related cognition in aged rats. However, using a bowl-digging version of the probabilistic reversal learning task, we found a deficit in cognitive flexibility in aged rats that did not translate across to a touchscreen version of the task. While these data reveal important changes in cognitive flexibility and anxiety associated with ageing, aged rats do not show deficits across other behavioural domains relevant to apathy. This suggests that aged rats are not a suitable model for age-related apathy syndrome. These findings contrast with previous work in mice, revealing important species differences in behaviours relevant to apathy syndrome in ageing.


Aging , Anxiety , Apathy , Disease Models, Animal , Motivation , Reward , Animals , Male , Apathy/physiology , Aging/physiology , Motivation/physiology , Anxiety/physiopathology , Rats , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Reversal Learning/physiology , Exploratory Behavior/physiology
4.
J Affect Disord ; 356: 414-423, 2024 Jul 01.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38640975

BACKGROUND: Amotivation is a typical feature in major depressive disorder (MDD), which produces reduced willingness to exert effort. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) is a crucial structure in goal-directed actions and therefore is a potential target in modulating effortful motivation. However, it remains unclear whether the intervention is effective for patients with MDD. METHODS: We employed transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), computational modelling and event-related potentials (ERPs) to reveal the causal relationship between the left DLPFC and motivation for effortful rewards in MDD. Fifty patients underwent both active and sham TMS sessions, each followed by performing an Effort-Expenditure for Rewards Task, during which participants chose and implemented between low-effort/low-reward and high-effort/high-reward options. RESULTS: The patients showed increased willingness to exert effort for rewards during the DLPFC facilitated session, compared with the sham session. They also had a trend in larger P3 amplitude for motivated attention toward chosen options, larger CNV during preparing for effort exertion, and larger SPN during anticipating a high reward. Besides, while behavior indexes for effortful choices were negatively related to depression severity in the sham session, this correlation was weakened in the active stimulation session. CONCLUSIONS: These findings provide behavioral, computational, and neural evidence for the left DLPFC on effortful motivation for rewards. Facilitated DLPFC improves motor preparation and value anticipation after making decisions especially for highly effortful rewards in MDD. Facilitated DLPFC also has a potential function in enhancing motivated attention during cost-benefit trade-off. This neuromodulation effect provides a potential treatment for improving motivation in clinics.


Depressive Disorder, Major , Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex , Motivation , Reward , Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation , Humans , Depressive Disorder, Major/therapy , Depressive Disorder, Major/physiopathology , Depressive Disorder, Major/psychology , Motivation/physiology , Male , Female , Adult , Middle Aged , Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Electroencephalography , Attention/physiology
5.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 19(1)2024 May 10.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38584414

Developments in cognitive neuroscience have led to the emergence of hyperscanning, the simultaneous measurement of brain activity from multiple people. Hyperscanning is useful for investigating social cognition, including joint action, because of its ability to capture neural processes that occur within and between people as they coordinate actions toward a shared goal. Here, we provide a practical guide for researchers considering using hyperscanning to study joint action and seeking to avoid frequently raised concerns from hyperscanning skeptics. We focus specifically on Electroencephalography (EEG) hyperscanning, which is widely available and optimally suited for capturing fine-grained temporal dynamics of action coordination. Our guidelines cover questions that are likely to arise when planning a hyperscanning project, ranging from whether hyperscanning is appropriate for answering one's research questions to considerations for study design, dependent variable selection, data analysis and visualization. By following clear guidelines that facilitate careful consideration of the theoretical implications of research design choices and other methodological decisions, joint action researchers can mitigate interpretability issues and maximize the benefits of hyperscanning paradigms.


Electroencephalography , Motivation , Humans , Electroencephalography/methods , Motivation/physiology , Brain/physiology , Research Design/standards , Social Cognition
6.
Behav Brain Res ; 467: 114996, 2024 Jun 05.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38609021

Motivational deficits and reduced goal-directed behavior for external rewards have long been considered an important features of negative symptoms in patients with schizophrenia (SCZ). Negative symptoms have also a high prevalence in bipolar disorder (BP). We used a transdiagnostic approach in order to examine association between negative symptoms and effort allocation for monetary rewards. 41 patients with SCZ and 34 patients with BP were enrolled in the study along with 41 healthy controls (HC). Effort-Expenditure for Rewards Task (EEfRT) was used to measure subjects' effort allocation for monetary rewards. Generalized estimating equation models were used to analyze EEfRT choice behavior. Negative symptoms were assessed using the Brief Negative Symptom Scale (BNSS). SCZ and BP groups expended lower effort to obtain a monetary rewards compared to HC. Severity of negative symptoms was negatively correlated with EEfRT performance in both diagnostic groups. Each diagnostic group showed lower effort allocation for monetary rewards compared to HC suggesting reduced motivation for monetary rewards. In addition, our results suggest that abnormal effort-based decision-making might be a transdiagnostic factor underlying negative symptoms.


Bipolar Disorder , Decision Making , Motivation , Reward , Schizophrenia , Schizophrenic Psychology , Humans , Bipolar Disorder/physiopathology , Male , Female , Adult , Decision Making/physiology , Schizophrenia/physiopathology , Motivation/physiology , Middle Aged , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Young Adult
7.
Cell Rep ; 43(4): 114027, 2024 Apr 23.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38568813

The insular cortex is involved in diverse processes, including bodily homeostasis, emotions, and cognition. However, we lack a comprehensive understanding of how it processes information at the level of neuronal populations. We leveraged recent advances in unsupervised machine learning to study insular cortex population activity patterns (i.e., neuronal manifold) in mice performing goal-directed behaviors. We find that the insular cortex activity manifold is remarkably consistent across different animals and under different motivational states. Activity dynamics within the neuronal manifold are highly stereotyped during rewarded trials, enabling robust prediction of single-trial outcomes across different mice and across various natural and artificial motivational states. Comparing goal-directed behavior with self-paced free consumption, we find that the stereotyped activity patterns reflect task-dependent goal-directed reward anticipation, and not licking, taste, or positive valence. These findings reveal a core computation in insular cortex that could explain its involvement in pathologies involving aberrant motivations.


Goals , Insular Cortex , Animals , Mice , Insular Cortex/physiology , Male , Motivation/physiology , Reward , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Neurons/physiology , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology
9.
Behav Processes ; 217: 105021, 2024 Apr.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38493969

Spatial and social cognition are two aspects of fish behaviour that have been subject to an increasing amount of research in recent years, but few have investigated potential behaviour overlaps. Testing the ability for an individual to socially learn a spatial task would bridge this gap in understanding. We provided naïve goldfish, Carassius auratus, the opportunity to observe a trained conspecific navigate a T-shaped maze, and then recorded how many trials it took for them to learn the maze, time taken per trial, motivation, and acceptance of the food reward. We also recorded how many trials it took a control group to learn the maze without the opportunity to observe a demonstrator. The observer group took significantly longer to learn the maze than the control group. Although the observer group were significantly less motivated (trials without a choice made), they were significantly more likely to accept the food reward. The social learning of reward acceptance was taking place, but the process of the demonstration disrupted the training of the spatial task, with possible explanations as the passenger effect and trade-off mechanism being discussed. Future studies are needed to determine whether goldfish can acquire spatial information socially; however, this study contributes to the feasibility of studying social learning of environmentally information in goldfish.


Goldfish , Motivation , Reward , Animals , Goldfish/physiology , Motivation/physiology , Maze Learning/physiology , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Social Behavior , Social Learning/physiology , Male
10.
Horm Behav ; 161: 105529, 2024 May.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38492501

Central to the navigation of an ever-changing environment is the ability to form positive associations with places and conspecifics. The functions of location and social conditioned preferences are often studied independently, limiting our understanding of their interplay. Furthermore, a de-emphasis on natural functions of conditioned preferences has led to neurobiological interpretations separated from ecological context. By adopting a naturalistic and ethological perspective, we uncover complexities underlying the expression of conditioned preferences. Development of conditioned preferences is a combination of motivation, reward, associative learning, and context, including for social and spatial environments. Both social- and location-dependent reward-responsive behaviors and their conditioning rely on internal state-gating mechanisms that include neuroendocrine and hormone systems such as opioids, dopamine, testosterone, estradiol, and oxytocin. Such reinforced behavior emerges from mechanisms integrating past experience and current social and environmental conditions. Moreover, social context, environmental stimuli, and internal state gate and modulate motivation and learning via associative reward, shaping the conditioning process. We highlight research incorporating these concepts, focusing on the integration of social neuroendocrine mechanisms and behavioral conditioning. We explore three paradigms: 1) conditioned place preference, 2) conditioned social preference, and 3) social conditioned place preference. We highlight nonclassical species to emphasize the naturalistic applications of these conditioned preferences. To fully appreciate the complex integration of spatial and social information, future research must identify neural networks where endocrine systems exert influence on such behaviors. Such research promises to provide valuable insights into conditioned preferences within a broader naturalistic context.


Reward , Animals , Motivation/physiology , Humans , Endocrine System/physiology , Social Behavior , Conditioning, Psychological/physiology , Association Learning/physiology
11.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 46(1): 6-15, 2024 Feb.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38299800

INTRODUCTION: Performance validity test (PVT) failures occur in clinical practice and at higher rates with external incentives. However, little PVT research has been applied to the Long COVID population. This study aims to address this gap. METHODS: Participants were 247 consecutive individuals with Long COVID seen for neuropsychological evaluation who completed 4 PVTs and a standardized neuropsychological battery. The sample was 84.2% White and 66% female. The mean age was 51.16 years and mean education was 14.75 years. Medical records were searched for external incentive (e.g., disability claims). Three groups were created based on PVT failures (Pass [no failures], Intermediate [1 failure], and Fail [2+ failures]). RESULTS: A total of 8.9% participants failed 2+ PVTs, 6.4% failed one PVT, and 85% passed PVTs. From the full sample, 25.1% were identified with external incentive. However, there was a significant difference between the rates of external incentives in the Fail group (54.5%) compared to the Pass (22.1%) and Intermediate (20%) groups. Further, the Fail group had lower cognitive scores and higher frequency of impaired range scores, consistent with PVT research in other populations. External incentives were uncorrelated with cognitive performance. CONCLUSIONS: Consistent with other populations, results suggest Long COVID cases are not immune to PVT failure and external incentives are associated with PVT failure. Results indicated that individuals in the Pass and Intermediate groups showed no evidence for significant cognitive deficits, but the Fail group had significantly poorer cognitive performance. Thus, PVTs should be routinely administered in Long COVID cases and research.


COVID-19 , Motivation , Neuropsychological Tests , Humans , Female , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests/standards , COVID-19/complications , Motivation/physiology , Adult , Aged , Post-Acute COVID-19 Syndrome , Cognitive Dysfunction/etiology , Cognitive Dysfunction/diagnosis , Cognitive Dysfunction/physiopathology , Cognition/physiology , Reproducibility of Results
12.
Br J Educ Psychol ; 94(2): 407-424, 2024 Jun.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38195085

BACKGROUND: Real-world problems are important in math instruction, but they do not necessarily trigger students' task motivation. Personalizing real-world problems by (1) matching problems to students' shared living environment (context personalization) and (2) asking students to pose their own problems (active personalization) might be two interventions to increase students' task motivation. AIM: In the current study, we investigated the effects of context personalization and active personalization on students' self-efficacy expectations, intrinsic value, attainment value, utility value, and cost. SAMPLE: The participants were 28 fifth- and sixth-grade students who voluntarily took part in a six-month afterschool program in which they posed problems with the aim of creating a math walk in their hometown. METHOD: Using a within-subjects design, at the end of the afterschool program, the students rated their self-efficacy expectations and task values for four self-developed problems associated with their hometown, four peer-developed problems associated with their hometown, and four instructor-provided problems associated with unfamiliar locations. RESULTS: Students reported higher self-efficacy expectations, intrinsic value, attainment value, and utility value for active-personalized than non-personalized problems. To a lesser extent, context personalization promoted intrinsic value and attainment value. No effect was found for cost. CONCLUSIONS: Active personalization (i.e. asking students to pose their own real-world problems) is suited to enhance students' task motivation, specifically their self-efficacy expectations, intrinsic value, attainment value, and utility value. Context personalization still boosts students' intrinsic value and attainment value. Implementation in classroom instruction is discussed.


Mathematics , Motivation , Self Efficacy , Students , Humans , Male , Female , Child , Students/psychology , Motivation/physiology , Mathematics/education
13.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci ; 24(2): 269-278, 2024 Apr.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38168850

Translation of drug targets from preclinical studies to clinical trials has been aided by cross-species behavioral tasks, but evidence for brain-based engagement during task performance is still required. Cross-species progressive ratio breakpoint tasks (PRBTs) measure motivation-related behavior and are pharmacologically and clinically sensitive. We recently advanced elevated parietal alpha power as a cross-species electroencephalographic (EEG) biomarker of PRBT engagement. Given that amphetamine increases breakpoint in mice, we tested its effects on breakpoint and parietal alpha power in both humans and mice. Twenty-three healthy participants performed the PRBT with EEG after amphetamine or placebo in a double-blind design. C57BL/6J mice were trained on PRBT with EEG (n = 24) and were treated with amphetamine or vehicle. A second cohort of mice was trained on PRBT without EEG (n = 40) and was treated with amphetamine or vehicle. In humans, amphetamine increased breakpoint. In mice, during concomitant EEG, 1 mg/kg of amphetamine significantly decreased breakpoint. In cohort 2, however, 0.3 mg/kg of amphetamine increased breakpoint consistent with human findings. Increased alpha power was observed in both species as they reached breakpoint, replicating previous findings. Amphetamine did not affect alpha power in either species. Amphetamine increased effort in humans and mice. Consistent with previous reports, elevated parietal alpha power was observed in humans and mice as they performed the PRBT. Amphetamine did not affect this EEG biomarker of effort. Hence, these findings support the pharmacological predictive validity of the PRBT to measure effort in humans and mice and suggest that this EEG biomarker is not directly reflective of amphetamine-induced changes in effort.


Amphetamine , Central Nervous System Stimulants , Electroencephalography , Mice, Inbred C57BL , Motivation , Amphetamine/pharmacology , Humans , Animals , Male , Electroencephalography/drug effects , Adult , Young Adult , Double-Blind Method , Motivation/drug effects , Motivation/physiology , Female , Central Nervous System Stimulants/pharmacology , Central Nervous System Stimulants/administration & dosage , Mice , Alpha Rhythm/drug effects , Alpha Rhythm/physiology
14.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci ; 24(3): 469-490, 2024 Jun.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38291308

Psychological research on human motivation repeatedly observed that approach goals (i.e., goals to attain success) increase task enjoyment and intrinsic motivation more strongly than avoidance goals (i.e., goals to avoid failure). The present study sought to address how the reward network in the brain-including the striatum and ventromedial prefrontal cortex-is involved when individuals engage in the same task with a focus on approach or avoidance goals. Participants reported stronger positive emotions when they focused on approach goals, but stronger anxiety and disappointment when they focused on avoidance goals. The fMRI analyses revealed that the reward network in the brain showed similar levels of activity to cues predictive of approach and avoidance goals. In contrast, the two goal states were associated with different patterns of activity in the visual cortex, hippocampus, and cerebellum during success and failure outcomes. Representation similarity analysis further revealed shared and different representations within the striatum and vmPFC between the approach and avoidance goal states, suggesting both the similarity and uniqueness of the mechanisms behind the two goal states. In addition, the distinct patterns of activation in the striatum were associated with distinct subjective experiences participants reported between the approach and the avoidance conditions. These results suggest the importance of examining the pattern of striatal activity in understanding the mechanisms behind different motivational states in humans.


Anxiety , Brain Mapping , Brain , Goals , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Motivation , Reward , Humans , Male , Female , Motivation/physiology , Young Adult , Anxiety/physiopathology , Brain/physiology , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Adult , Avoidance Learning/physiology , Happiness , Adolescent
15.
Science ; 382(6669): 394-398, 2023 10 27.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37883553

The nervous system coordinates various motivated behaviors such as feeding, drinking, and escape to promote survival and evolutionary fitness. Although the precise behavioral repertoires required for distinct motivated behaviors are diverse, common features such as approach or avoidance suggest that common brain substrates are required for a wide range of motivated behaviors. In this Review, I describe a framework by which neural circuits specified for some innate drives regulate the activity of ventral tegmental area (VTA) dopamine neurons to reinforce ongoing or planned actions to fulfill motivational demands. This framework may explain why signaling from VTA dopamine neurons is ubiquitously involved in many types of diverse volitional motivated actions, as well as how sensory and interoceptive cues can initiate specific goal-directed actions.


Dopaminergic Neurons , Motivation , Reward , Ventral Tegmental Area , Dopaminergic Neurons/physiology , Motivation/physiology , Signal Transduction , Ventral Tegmental Area/physiology , Animals
16.
Drug Alcohol Depend ; 246: 109852, 2023 05 01.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37003108

Research suggests that disproportionate exposure to risk factors places American Indian (AI) peoples at higher risk for substance use disorders (SUD). Although SUD is linked to striatal prioritization of drug rewards over other appetitive stimuli, there are gaps in the literature related to the investigation of aversive valuation processing, and inclusion of AI samples. To address these gaps, this study compared striatal anticipatory gain and loss processing between AI-identified with SUD (SUD+; n = 52) and without SUD (SUD-; n = 35) groups from the Tulsa 1000 study who completed a monetary incentive delay (MID) task during functional magnetic resonance imaging. Results indicated that striatal activations in the nucleus accumbens (NAcc), caudate, and putamen were greatest for anticipating gains (ps < 0.001) but showed no group differences. In contrast to gains, the SUD+ exhibited lower NAcc (p = .01, d =0.53) and putamen (p = .04, d =0.40) activation to anticipating large losses than the comparison group. Within SUD+ , lower striatal responses during loss anticipations were associated with slower MID reaction times (NAcc: r = -0.43; putamen: r = -0.35) during loss trials. This is among the first imaging studies to examine underlying neural mechanisms associated with SUD within AIs. Attenuated loss processing provides initial evidence of a potential mechanism wherein blunted prediction of aversive consequences may be a defining feature of SUD that can inform future prevention and intervention targets.


American Indian or Alaska Native , Anticipation, Psychological , Corpus Striatum , Economic Factors , Substance-Related Disorders , Humans , American Indian or Alaska Native/psychology , Anticipation, Psychological/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Motivation/physiology , Nucleus Accumbens/diagnostic imaging , Nucleus Accumbens/physiopathology , Reward , Substance-Related Disorders/diagnostic imaging , Substance-Related Disorders/economics , Substance-Related Disorders/ethnology , Substance-Related Disorders/psychology , Urban Population , Risk Factors , Corpus Striatum/diagnostic imaging , Corpus Striatum/physiopathology , Income
17.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 49(1): 145-158, 2023 Jan.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36395053

Habituation to onset distractors has been shown to be stronger the higher the distractor probability. However, since in previous studies distractor probability covaried with distractor numerosity, it was unclear whether habituation was controlled by a mechanism that relies on distractor expectation (Sokolov, 1963), or by a mechanism that is merely driven by the number of stimulations delivered to the nervous system (Groves & Thompson, 1970). To address this issue, we manipulated the probability of distractor occurrence at a fixed location, without varying the number of distractors being presented. The results of Experiment 1 clearly favored the Sokolov model of habituation, showing that habituation of capture is controlled by the level of distractor expectation for the same distractors number. Experiment 2 excluded that the pattern of habituation was determined by the difference in the temporal frequency of the distractor between higher and lower distractor rates. Furthermore, the results of Experiment 3 suggested that the amount of habituation of capture is mainly controlled by the local rather than by the global rate of the onset distractor occurrence, thus indicating that habituation of capture is largely spatially specific. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).


Attention , Habituation, Psychophysiologic , Humans , Attention/physiology , Habituation, Psychophysiologic/physiology , Motivation/physiology , Probability , Reaction Time/physiology
18.
Int. j. psychol. psychol. ther. (Ed. impr.) ; 22(3): 243-263, oct. 2022. ilus, tab
Article En | IBECS | ID: ibc-209993

Since the beginning of experimental studies in psychology, there has been a keen interest in analyzing the factors that distort our perception of time as human beings. The manipulation of emotional or/and motivational states has been critical to understanding how an individual’s perception of time changes. However, experimental studies to date have been highly diverse in both their procedures and the instruments used to measure such perceptions. This paper proposes a systematic review of the topic in question. The main aim of this work is to describe and compare different experimental studies conducted with adults. After rigorous selection of search terms to introduce in each database, inclusion criteria were applied while some articles were added through citation search. A total of 17 articles with 26 experimental studies were analyzed. The results were discussed, and further research should be advanced (AU)


Humans , Reaction Time/physiology , Perception/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Motivation/physiology
19.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 119(5)2022 02 01.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35101919

Current models of mental effort in psychology, behavioral economics, and cognitive neuroscience typically suggest that exerting cognitive effort is aversive, and people avoid it whenever possible. The aim of this research was to challenge this view and show that people can learn to value and seek effort intrinsically. Our experiments tested the hypothesis that effort-contingent reward in a working-memory task will induce a preference for more demanding math tasks in a transfer phase, even though participants were aware that they would no longer receive any reward for task performance. In laboratory Experiment 1 (n = 121), we made reward directly contingent on mobilized cognitive effort as assessed via cardiovascular measures (ß-adrenergic sympathetic activity) during the training task. Experiments 2a to 2e (n = 1,457) were conducted online to examine whether the effects of effort-contingent reward on subsequent demand seeking replicate and generalize to community samples. Taken together, the studies yielded reliable evidence that effort-contingent reward increased participants' demand seeking and preference for the exertion of cognitive effort on the transfer task. Our findings provide evidence that people can learn to assign positive value to mental effort. The results challenge currently dominant theories of mental effort and provide evidence and an explanation for the positive effects of environments appreciating effort and individual growth on people's evaluation of effort and their willingness to mobilize effort and approach challenging tasks.


Achievement , Cognition/physiology , Learning/physiology , Adult , Decision Making/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Memory, Short-Term , Mental Processes/physiology , Motivation/physiology , Reward , Social Values , Task Performance and Analysis , Young Adult
20.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 2832, 2022 02 18.
Article En | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35181696

The main hypothesis for the relation between physical activity and mental health is that autonomous motivation, such as subjective pleasure for the activity, plays an important role. However, no report has described empirical research designed to examine the role of subjective pleasure in the relation between objectively measured physical activity and psychological indexes. We used accelerometers to collect data indicating participants' physical activity intensity during a week. Participants recorded their subjective pleasure of activity per hour. In 69% of them, the individual correlation coefficients between physical activity and pleasure in an hour (an index of Physical Activity-Pleasure; PA-PL) were positive (r = 0.22, 95%Cl = [0.11-0.38]), indicating that pleasant sensations increased concomitantly with increasing physical activity. Conversely, 31% participants exhibited negative values of PA-PL, which means that the increase in physical activity had the opposite effect, decreasing pleasure. Multiple linear regression analysis showed that avoidance/rumination behaviors decreased significantly with increased PA-PL (ß = -6.82, 95%CI: [-13.27 to -0.38], p < .05). These results indicate that subjective pleasure attached to the PA is more important than the PA amount for reducing depressive behavior.


Avoidance Learning/physiology , Exercise/psychology , Motivation/physiology , Pleasure , Adolescent , Emotions/physiology , Exercise/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Health , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
...